Parents’ epistemological beliefs shape children’s scientific reasoning abilities, study suggests

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

A recent study published in the journal Developmental Science has explored how parents’ views on knowledge and learning (epistemological beliefs) influence their children’s ability to think scientifically from kindergarten through elementary school. The findings suggest that these parental beliefs significantly impact children’s early scientific reasoning abilities, irrespective of the parents’ level of education or the children’s own cognitive abilities.

Scientific reasoning is crucial for effective problem-solving and decision-making in our increasingly complex world. Previous studies have shown that children can exhibit sophisticated scientific reasoning skills at a young age, influenced by their understanding of the relationship between hypotheses and evidence. While the effect of educators’ and students’ epistemological beliefs on their teaching and learning has been well-documented, less is known about the impact of parents’ beliefs on their children’s development in this area.

“We got interested in this topic because we noticed that some kids are really good at scientific thinking from a young age, while others struggle more. We wanted to figure out why that is,” said study author Christopher Osterhaus, a professor of developmental psychology in education at the University of Vechta.

“So, we looked into how parents’ ideas about knowledge – what they think about science and what we can know – might influence how well their kids grasp scientific concepts. We studied 161 German kids aged 6 to 10 over five years to see how their parents’ beliefs affected their scientific thinking. Our long-term goal is to find out how we can help all kids, no matter where they start, to become better at thinking like scientists.”

The families were recruited from both urban and rural settings to ensure a wide range of socioeconomic backgrounds and educational levels. The children and parents in the study were selected through kindergartens in a midsized university city and in rural areas, adding diversity to the sample which included 89 girls and 72 boys.

For children, the researchers used two main instruments designed to gauge scientific reasoning at different educational stages. For the younger children in kindergarten to second grade, the Science-Kindergarten Inventory (SK-I) was employed. This tool included items that assessed basic scientific processes like experimentation and data interpretation. As the children grew older, the study introduced the Science-Primary School Reasoning Inventory (SPR-I), which contained more advanced items suitable for elementary school students.

Parents’ epistemological beliefs were assessed using a questionnaire adapted from the Student Understanding of Science and Scientific Inquiry (SUSSI). This instrument includes a series of statements rated on a Likert scale, where parents expressed their level of agreement or disagreement with statements about science. The focus was on three key areas: the interpretative nature of scientific observations, the changeability of scientific theories, and the impact of social and cultural contexts on scientific practices.

Example items included: “Scientific theories may be completely replaced by new theories in light of new evidence” and “Scientists’ observations of the same event may be different because the scientists’ prior knowledge may affect their observations.”

One of the key findings was that the epistemological beliefs held by parents significantly predicted the scientific reasoning abilities of their children. This effect was consistent across all data points collected during the study and was independent of other variables such as the parents’ education levels or the children’s general cognitive abilities. This indicates that what parents think about knowledge and learning can substantially shape their children’s scientific reasoning skills from an early age.

“The main takeaway from our study is that parents play a crucial role in shaping their children’s ability to think scientifically,” Osterhaus told PsyPost. “It’s not just about what kids learn in school; it’s also about the beliefs and attitudes they pick up from their parents at home. So, for the average person, it’s important to recognize that fostering a supportive environment for scientific exploration and inquiry in the home can greatly enhance a child’s scientific thinking skills. By being aware of the influence they have, parents and caregivers can actively contribute to their child’s development in this critical area.”

Interestingly, while parental epistemological beliefs were found to significantly influence the initial levels of children’s scientific reasoning abilities, they did not affect the rate at which these abilities developed over time. Essentially, children whose parents held more nuanced and sophisticated views on science started at a higher baseline in scientific reasoning but did not necessarily advance faster than their peers.

The researchers also examined which specific aspects of epistemology were most influential. The findings revealed that the parents’ beliefs about the social and cultural influences on science (framework theories) were particularly predictive of the children’s scientific reasoning skills. This dimension of epistemology had a stronger impact compared to beliefs about the interpretative nature of observations or the changeability of scientific theories.

The influence of parental epistemological beliefs was specific to scientific reasoning, with no significant effects found on other cognitive areas such as language skills or general intelligence. This specificity underscores the unique role that parents’ beliefs about knowledge play in the context of scientific reasoning, distinct from other forms of cognitive development.

“One surprising aspect of our findings was the extent to which parental beliefs about knowledge didn’t just affect how well children did on specific tasks, but also influenced their overall development of scientific thinking over a five-year period,” Osterhaus said. “We were struck by the consistency of this influence across time, highlighting the enduring impact of parental epistemology on children’s cognitive growth in the realm of science. This underscores the need to consider long-term effects when designing interventions aimed at fostering scientific thinking in children.”

But the study, like all research, includes limitations. “A caveat of our study is its focus on German children, which limits the generalizability of our findings to other cultural and socioeconomic contexts,” Osterhaus noted. “While we accounted for factors such as parental education and socioeconomic status, further research is needed to explore how parental epistemology influences scientific thinking in children across diverse cultural backgrounds.”

“Our long-term goal for this line of research is to inform educational practices and interventions aimed at enhancing children’s scientific thinking skills,” the researcher explained. “By integrating parental epistemological understanding into educational programs, we aim to ensure that all children have the opportunity to fully develop their scientific potential. Ultimately, we hope our findings contribute to creating more inclusive and effective approaches to science education that empower children from diverse backgrounds to engage confidently and proficiently in scientific inquiry.”

Overall, the researchers demonstrated that the way parents perceive and convey the nature of knowledge and learning can have a lasting impact on their children’s ability to reason scientifically. This effect begins early in childhood and establishes a baseline that carries through elementary school, emphasizing the importance of nurturing thoughtful and critical perspectives on science within the family environment.

“Our findings underscore the importance of engaging parents and caregivers as partners in promoting scientific thinking in children,” Osterhaus concluded. “By raising awareness about the influence of parental beliefs on children’s cognitive development, we hope to spark conversations within families about the value of fostering a supportive environment for scientific inquiry at home. This dialogue can empower parents to play an active role in nurturing their children’s curiosity, critical thinking, and problem-solving skills, ultimately laying a solid foundation for lifelong learning and success in the 21st century.”

The study, “The personal epistemology of parents predicts the development of scientific reasoning in children aged 6–10 years,” was authored by Christopher Osterhaus and Susanne Koerber.

© PsyPost